Letter

Nature 461, 784-787 (8 October 2009) | doi:10.1038/nature08401; Received 19 June 2009; Accepted 14 August 2009; Published online 16 September 2009

Gene therapy for red–green colour blindness in adult primates

Katherine Mancuso1, William W. Hauswirth2, Qiuhong Li2, Thomas B. Connor3, James A. Kuchenbecker1, Matthew C. Mauck3, Jay Neitz1 & Maureen Neitz1

  1. Department of Ophthalmology, Box 356485, University of Washington, 1959 North East Pacific Street, Seattle, Washington 98195, USA
  2. Department of Ophthalmology and Powell Gene Therapy Center, University of Florida, 1600 South West Archer Road, Gainesville, Florida 32610, USA
  3. Department of Ophthalmology, Medical College of Wisconsin, 925 North 87th Street, Milwaukee, Wisconsin 53226, USA

Correspondence to: Jay Neitz1 Correspondence and requests for materials should be addressed to J.N. (Email: jneitz@uw.edu).

Top

Red–green colour blindness, which results from the absence of either the long- (L) or the middle- (M) wavelength-sensitive visual photopigments, is the most common single locus genetic disorder. Here we explore the possibility of curing colour blindness using gene therapy in experiments on adult monkeys that had been colour blind since birth. A third type of cone pigment was added to dichromatic retinas, providing the receptoral basis for trichromatic colour vision. This opened a new avenue to explore the requirements for establishing the neural circuits for a new dimension of colour sensation. Classic visual deprivation experiments1 have led to the expectation that neural connections established during development would not appropriately process an input that was not present from birth. Therefore, it was believed that the treatment of congenital vision disorders would be ineffective unless administered to the very young. However, here we show that the addition of a third opsin in adult red–green colour-deficient primates was sufficient to produce trichromatic colour vision behaviour. Thus, trichromacy can arise from a single addition of a third cone class and it does not require an early developmental process. This provides a positive outlook for the potential of gene therapy to cure adult vision disorders.

Gene therapy was performed on adult squirrel monkeys (Saimiri sciureus) that were missing the L-opsin gene. In this species, some females have trichromatic colour vision whereas males are red–green colour blind2. Serotype 2/5 recombinant adeno-associated virus (rAAV) containing a human L-opsin gene under the control of the L/M-opsin enhancer and promoter (Fig. 1a) was delivered to the photoreceptor layer by subretinal injections (see Methods). Transcriptional regulatory elements were chosen to direct expression preferentially in M cones, but not short- (S) wavelength-sensitive cones or rods3. To provide the receptoral basis for trichromacy, animals received three 100-µl injections (containing a total of 2.7×1013 viral particles) in each eye, which produced a relatively uniform, third submosaic of approximately 15–36% of M cones that coexpressed the transgene (Fig. 1e, f).

Figure 1: rAAV2/5 vector produced functional L-opsin in primate retina.
Figure 1 : rAAV2/5 vector produced functional L-opsin in primate 
retina. Unfortunately we are unable to provide accessible alternative 
text for this. If you require assistance to access this image, or to 
obtain a text description, please contact npg@nature.com

a, Molecular map. LCR,locus control region; PA1, polyadenylation signal; PP,proximal promoter; RHLOPS,recombinant human L-opsin cDNA; SD/SA,splice donor/acceptor; TR,terminal repeats. b, Red light mf-ERG stimulus. c, mf-ERG 40weeks after two injections (yellow circles) of a mixture of L-opsin- and GFP-coding viruses. Grey lines show borders of highest response. For comparison, the insetshowsmf-ERG 16weeks after injection; there was no reliable signal from L-opsin, unchanged from baseline. High responses in far peripheral retina were measured reliably and may have originated from offshoot of one of the injections. d, Fluorescence photographs from a similar retinal area as in c; grey lines from c were copied in d. e, Confocal microscopy showed a mosaic pattern of GFP expression in 5–12% of cones. Because GFP-coding virus was diluted to one-third compared to L-opsin virus, an estimated 15–36% of cones in behaviourally tested animals express L-opsin. f, Mf-ERG from a behaviourally tested animal 70weeks after three injections of L-opsin virus.

High resolution image and legend (410K)Download PowerPoint slide (793K)

Slides may be downloaded for educational use, according to the terms described in Nature Publishing Group's licensing policy.


Before treatment, monkeys were trained to perform a computer-based colour vision test, the Cambridge Colour Test4, 5, which was modified for use with animals6 (Fig. 2a). Dichromats who are missing either the L- or the M-photopigment fail to distinguish from grey: colours near the so-called ‘spectral neutral point’ located in the blue-green region of colour space (near dominant wavelength of 490nm) and complementary colours near the ‘extra-spectral neutral point’ in the red-violet region (near dominant wavelength of-499nm). Whereas trichromats have the four main hue percepts blue, yellow, red and green, dichromats only have two percepts, nominally blue and yellow. Before treatment, two dichromatic monkeys completed three colour vision tests consisting of 16 hues (Fig. 2b, c). Four-to-six months were required to test all 16 hues; thus, baseline results represent testing conducted for more than a year. As predicted, before treatment monkeys had low thresholds (averaging <0.03 units in u′, v′ colour space) for colours that represent blues and yellows to their eyes, but always failed to discriminate between the blue-green and the red-violet (dominant wavelengths of490nm and -499nm, respectively) hues, with thresholds extrapolated from psychometric functions being orders of magnitude higher (Fig. 2b, c). Results were highly repeatable, with no improvement between the first and third tests, making us confident that the animals would not spontaneously improve in the absence of treatment.

Figure 2: Pre-therapy colour vision and possible treatment outcomes.
Figure 2 : Pre-therapy colour vision and possible treatment 
outcomes. Unfortunately we are unable to provide accessible alternative 
text for this. If you require assistance to access this image, or to 
obtain a text description, please contact npg@nature.com

a, Colour-vision stimuli examples. b, Pre-therapy results, monkey 1. Hues tested are represented as dominant wavelengths rather than u′, v′ coordinates. If a hue could not be reliably distinguished at even the highest saturation, the extrapolated threshold approached infinity. c, Pre-therapy results, monkey 2. d, e, Possible experimental outcomes: monkeys could have a relative increase in long-wavelength sensitivity, but remain dichromatic (dashed lines, d); theoretical colour spectrum appearances for a dichromat and a possible ‘spectral shift’ are shown. Alternatively, dichromatic monkeys could become trichromatic. Results from a trichromatic female control monkey are plotted (dashed line, e). Error bars denote s.e.m.; n varied from 7–11.

High resolution image and legend (322K)Download PowerPoint slide (704K)

Slides may be downloaded for educational use, according to the terms described in Nature Publishing Group's licensing policy.


Co-expressing the L-opsin transgene within a subset of endogenous M-cones shifted their spectral sensitivity to respond to long wavelength light, thus producing two distinct cone types absorbing in the middle-to-long wavelengths, as required for trichromacy. The spectral sensitivity shift was readily detected using a custom-built wide-field colour multifocal electroretinogram (mf-ERG) system (Fig. 1b, c, f) (see ref. 7 for details). In preliminary experiments, validity of the colour mf-ERG was tested using an animal that had received a mixture of the L-opsin-coding virus plus an identical virus, except that a green fluorescent protein (GFP) gene replaced the L-opsin gene. As reported previously, faint GFP fluorescence was first detected at 9weeks post-injection, and it continued to increase in area and intensity over 24weeks8. Although faint signs of GFP were first detectable at 9weeks, L-opsin levels sufficient to produce suprathreshold mf-ERG signals were still not present at 16weeks post-injection (Fig. 1c, inset). After GFP fluorescence became robust, the red light mf-ERG, which indicates responses from the introduced L-opsin, showed highly increased response amplitudes in two areas (Fig. 1c) corresponding to locations of subretinal injections (Fig. 1d).

The two dichromatic monkeys who participated in behavioural tests of colour vision were treated with L-opsin-coding virus only. Although the elongated pattern produced by two injections in Fig. 1c, d allowed mf-ERG validation, the treatment goal was to produce a homogeneous region, as resulted from three injections shown in Fig. 1f, in which the highest mf-ERG response covered about 80° of the central retina—roughly the area for which humans have good red–green discrimination. These results demonstrate that gene therapy changed the spectral sensitivity of a subset of the cones. A priori, there were two possibilities for how a change in spectral sensitivity might change colour vision behaviour. First, animals may have an increase in sensitivity to long-wavelength light, but if the neural circuitry for extracting colour information from the nascent ‘M+L cone’ submosaic was absent, they would remain dichromatic—the hallmark of which is having two hues that are indistinguishable from grey (Fig. 2d). The spectral neutral point for individuals that have only S and M cones (for example, monkeys 1 and 2 pre-therapy) occurs near the dominant wavelength of495nm. At the limit, an increase in spectral sensitivity would shift the monkeys’ neutral point towards that of individuals with only S and L cones, near the dominant wavelength of 505nm (Fig. 2d, dashed blue lines). The second, more engaging possibility was that treatment would be sufficient to expand sensory capacity in monkeys, providing them with trichromatic vision. In this case, the animals’ post-therapy results would appear similar to Fig. 2e, obtained from a trichromatic female control monkey.

Daily testing continued after treatment. After about 20weeks post-injection (Fig. 3a, arrow), the trained monkeys’ thresholds for blue-green and red-violet (dominant wavelengths of 490 and -499nm, respectively; Fig. 3b, c) improved, reducing to an average of 0.08units in u′, v′ colour space, indicating that they gained trichromatic vision. This time point corresponded to the same period in which robust levels of transgene expression were reported in the squirrel monkey8. A trichromatic female monkey and untreated dichromatic monkeys were tested in parallel. As expected, the female had low thresholds for all colours, averaging <0.03units in u′, v′ colour space, but the untreated dichromats always failed to discriminate between dominant wavelengths of490nm (Fig. 3a, triangle) and -499nm, indicating a clear difference between treated and untreated monkeys.

Figure 3: Gene therapy produced trichromatic colour vision.
Figure 3 : Gene therapy produced trichromatic colour vision. 
Unfortunately we are unable to provide accessible alternative text for 
this. If you require assistance to access this image, or to obtain a 
text description, please contact npg@nature.com

a, Time course of thresholds for the blue-green confusion colour, dominant wavelength of 490nm (circles), and a yellowish colour, dominant wavelength of554nm (squares). A logarithmic scale was used to fit high thresholds for the dominant wavelength of 490nm; significant improvement occurred after 20weeks. Enclosed data pointsdenoteuntreated dichromatic monkey thresholds, dominant wavelengths of 490nm (triangle) and 554nm (diamond). b, c, Comparison of pre-therapy (open circles, solid line) and post-therapy (solid dots, dashed line) thresholds. Enclosed data points are dominant wavelength 490nm thresholds when tested against a red-violet background (dominant wavelength of-499nm); pink trianglesshowtrichromatic female control thresholds. Error barsrepresent s.e.m.; n varied from 7–11.

High resolution image and legend (250K)Download PowerPoint slide (632K)

Slides may be downloaded for educational use, according to the terms described in Nature Publishing Group's licensing policy.


Early experiments in which we obtained negative results served as ‘sham controls’, demonstrating that acquiring a new dimension of colour vision requires a shift in spectral sensitivity that results from expression of an L pigment in a subset of M cones. Using similar subretinal injection procedures, we delivered fewer viral particles of an L-opsin-coding rAAV2/5 virus with an extra 146-base-pair (bp) segment near the splice donor/acceptor site that had been carried over from the cloning vector and that was absent in the GFP-coding rAAV2/5 virus. The 146-bp segment contained an ATG and a duplicate messenger RNA start site that may have interfered with expression (see Methods). Three monkeys received injections of this vector, containing an average of 1.7×1012 virus particles per eye, and no reliable changes in spectral sensitivity were measured using the ERG. One animal was also tested behaviourally and his colour vision was unchanged from baseline 1year after injection. In subsequent experiments reported here, we removed the extra 146-bp segment and also increased the amount of viral particles delivered per eye by approximately 16-fold, to 2.7×1013. Negative results from earlier injections demonstrated that the subretinal injection procedure itself does not produce changes in the ERG or in colour vision.

The change in spectral sensitivity measured with the mf-ERG is necessary but not sufficient to produce a new colour vision capacity. For example, individuals with L but no M cones (termed deuteranopes) have a relatively enhanced sensitivity to red light, but they are still as dichromatic as individuals with M but no L cones (protanopes) in that they are unable to distinguish particular ‘colours’ from grey. To verify that the behavioural change observed in animals expressing the L pigment transgene was not purely a shift in spectral sensitivity (see Fig. 2d), monkey 1 was also tested on dominant wavelengths of 496 and 500nm, and monkey 2 was tested on dominant wavelengths of 496 and 507nm. Together, these dominant wavelengths span the possible confusion points for deuteranopes and protanopes and for any intermediate dichromatic forms that could arise from expressing combinations of L and M pigments. As shown in Fig. 3b, c, both monkeys’ measured thresholds for these extra hues were similar to their thresholds for a dominant wavelength of 490nm, demonstrating that they now lacked a spectral neutral point and have become truly trichromatic. Furthermore, treated monkeys were able to discriminate blue-green (dominant wavelength of 490nm) when it was tested against a red-violet (dominant wavelength of -499nm) background, instead of the grey background, indicating that the monkeys’ newly-acquired ‘green’ and ‘red’ percepts were distinct from one another. The treated monkeys’ improvement in colour vision has remained stable for more than 2years and we plan to continue testing the animals to evaluate long-term treatment effects.

Classic experiments in which visual deprivation of one eye during development caused permanent vision loss1 led to the idea that inputs must be present during development for the formation of circuits to process them. From the clear change in behaviour associated with treatment, compared both between and within subjects, we conclude that adult monkeys gained new colour vision capacities because of gene therapy. These startling empirical results provide insight into the evolutionary question of what changes in the visual system are required for adding a new dimension of colour vision. Previously, it seemed possible that a transformation from dichromacy to trichromacy would require evolutionary/developmental changes, in addition to acquiring a third cone type. For example, L- and M-opsin-specific genetic regulatory elements might have been required to direct the opsins into distinct cone types9 that would be recognized by L- and M-cone-specific retinal circuitry10, and to account for cortical processing, multi-stage circuitry11 might have evolved specifically for the purpose of trichromacy. However, our results demonstrate that trichromatic colour vision behaviour requires nothing more than a third cone type. As an alternative to the idea that the new dimension of colour vision arose by acquisition of a new L versus M pathway, it is possible that it exploited the pre-existing blue-yellow circuitry. For example, if the addition of the third cone class split the formerly S versus M receptive fields into two types with differing spectral sensitivities, this would obviate the need for neural rewiring as part of the process of adopting new colour vision.

Some form of inherent plasticity in the mammalian visual system can be inferred from the acquisition of new colour vision, as was also demonstrated in genetically engineered mice12; however, the point has been made that such plasticity need not indicate that any rewiring of the neural circuitry has occurred13. Similarly, given the fact that new colour vision behaviour in adult squirrel monkeys corresponded to the same time interval as the appearance of robust levels of transgene expression, we conclude that rewiring of the visual system was not associated with the change from dichromatic to trichromatic vision.

Treated adult monkeys unquestionably respond to colours that were previously invisible to them. The internal experiences associated with the marked change in discrimination thresholds measured here cannot be determined; therefore, we cannot know whether the animals experience new internal sensations of red and green. Nonetheless, we do know that evolution acts on behaviour, not on internalized experiences, and we suggest that gene therapy recapitulated what occurred during evolution of trichromacy in primates. These experiments demonstrate that a new colour-vision capacity, as defined by new discrimination abilities, can be added by taking advantage of pre-existing neural circuitry and, internal experience aside, full colour vision could have evolved in the absence of any other change in the visual system except the addition of a third cone type.

Gene therapy trials are underway for Leber’s congenital amaurosis14, 15, 16. Thus far, treatment has been administered to individuals who have suffered retinal degeneration from the disease. The experiments reported here are, to our knowledge, the first to use gene therapy in primates to address a vision disorder in which all photoreceptors are intact and healthy, making it possible to assess the full potential of gene therapy to restore visual capacities. Treatment allowing monkeys to see new colours in adulthood provides a striking counter-example to what occurs under conditions of monocular deprivation. For instance, it is impossible to restore vision in an adult who had grown up with a unilateral cataract. Future technologies will allow many opportunities for functions to be added or restored in the eye. Although some changes may produce outcomes analogous to monocular deprivation, we predict that others, like gene therapy for red–green colour blindness, will provide vision where there was previously blindness.

Top

Methods Summary

Confocal microscopy

The animal in Fig. 1c, d succumbed to respiratory illness, unrelated to gene therapy, approximately 2years and 3months after injection. The retina was fixed in 4% paraformaldehyde in PBS, and rinsed in PBS with 10% and 30% sucrose. It was sequentially incubated with 10% normal donkey serum, rabbit monoclonal antibody to M/L-opsin (Chemicon, AB5405), and a Cy3 (red)-conjugated donkey anti-rabbit antibody (Jackson Immunoresearch). Confocal images were analysed using ImageJ (http://rsbweb.nih.gov). In the middle panel of Fig. 1e, magenta dots mark cone locations, and the red anti-M/L-opsin antibody staining was removed to show GFP-expressing (green) cells more clearly.

Behavioural colour vision assessment

A three-alternative forced-choice model in which position and saturation of the stimulus was randomized between trials was used. Monkeys had to discriminate the location of a coloured patch of dots that varied in size and brightness, surrounded by similarly varying grey dots. When animals touched the coloured target, a positive tone sounded and a juice reward was given; the next stimulus appeared immediately. (The squirrel monkey shown in Fig. 2c is drinking a reward from a previous trial). If the wrong position was chosen, a negative tone sounded, and a 2–3-s ‘penalty time’ occurred before the next trial.

For each hue, monkeys were tested on up to 11 different saturations ranging from 0.01 to 0.11 in u′, v′ colour space (CIE 1976) and a threshold was calculated, which was taken as the saturation required to reach a criterion of 57% correct, the value determined to be significantly greater than chance (33% correct, P = 0.05); see ref. 6 for full details. All procedures were conducted in accordance with the guidelines of the US National Institutes of Health about the care and use of animals.

Full methods accompany this paper.

Top

References

  1. Wiesel, T. N. & Hubel, D. H. Single-cell responses in striate cortex of kittens deprived of vision in one eye. J. Neurophysiol. 26, 1003–1017 (1963) | PubMed | ISI | ChemPort |
  2. Jacobs, G. H. A perspective on color vision in platyrrhine monkeys. Vision Res. 38, 3307–3313 (1998) | Article | PubMed | ISI | ChemPort |
  3. Li, Q., Timmers, A. M., Guy, J., Pang, J. & Hauswirth, W. W. Cone-specific expression using a human red opsin promoter in recombinant AAV. Vision Res. 48, 332–338 (2007) | Article | PubMed | ChemPort |
  4. Reffin, J. P., Astell, S. & Mollon, J. D. in Colour Vision Deficiencies X (eds Drum, B., Moreland, J. D. and Serra, A.) 69–76 (Kluwer Academic Publishers, 1991)
  5. Regan, B. C., Reffin, J. P. & Mollon, J. D. Luminance noise and the rapid determination of discrimination ellipses in colour deficiency. Vision Res. 34, 1279–1299 (1994) | Article | PubMed | ChemPort |
  6. Mancuso, K., Neitz, M. & Neitz, J. An adaptation of the Cambridge Colour Test for use with animals. Vis. Neurosci. 23, 695–701 (2006) | Article | PubMed
  7. Kuchenbecker, J. A., Sahay, M., Tait, D. M., Neitz, M. & Neitz, J. Topography of the long- to middle-wavelength sensitive cone ratio in the human retina assessed with a wide-field color multifocal electroretinogram. Vis. Neurosci. 25, 301–306 (2008) | Article | PubMed
  8. Mancuso, K. et al. Recombinant adeno-associated virus targets passenger gene expression to cones in primate retina. J. Opt. Soc. Am. A Opt. Image Sci. Vis. 24, 1411–1416 (2007) | Article | PubMed
  9. Nathans, J., Piantanida, T. P., Eddy, R. L., Shows, T. B. & Hogness, D. S. Molecular genetics of inherited variation in human color vision. Science 232, 203–210 (1986) | Article | PubMed | ISI | ChemPort |
  10. Shapley, R. Specificity of cone connections in the retina and color vision. Focus on "Specificity of cone inputs to macaque retinal ganglion cells". J. Neurophysiol. 95, 587–588 (2006) | Article | PubMed
  11. De Valois, R. L. & De Valois, K. K. A multi-stage color model. Vision Res. 33, 1053–1065 (1993) | Article | PubMed | ChemPort |
  12. Jacobs, G. H., Williams, G. A., Cahill, H. & Nathans, J. Emergence of novel color vision in mice engineered to express a human cone photopigment. Science 315, 1723–1725 (2007) | Article | PubMed | ChemPort |
  13. Makous, W. Comment on "emergence of novel color vision in mice engineered to express a human cone photopigment". Science 318, 196 (2007) | Article | PubMed
  14. Maguire, A. M. et al. Safety and efficacy of gene transfer for Leber's congenital amaurosis. N. Engl. J. Med. 358, 2240–2248 (2008) | Article | PubMed | ChemPort |
  15. Bainbridge, J. W. & Ali, R. R. Success in sight: the eyes have it! Ocular gene therapy trials for LCA look promising. Gene Ther. 15, 1191–1192 (2008) | Article | PubMed | ChemPort |
  16. Cideciyan, A. V. et al. Human gene therapy for RPE65 isomerase deficiency activates the retinoid cycle of vision but with slow rod kinetics. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 105, 15112–15117 (2008) | Article | PubMed
  17. Wang, Y. et al. A locus control region adjacent to the human red and green visual pigment genes. Neuron 9, 429–440 (1992) | Article | PubMed | ISI | ChemPort |
  18. Mauck, M. C. et al. Longitudinal evaluation of expression of virally delivered transgenes in gerbil cone photoreceptors. Vis. Neurosci. 25, 273–282 (2008) | Article | PubMed
  19. Nathans, J., Thomas, D. & Hogness, D. S. Molecular genetics of human color vision: the genes encoding blue, green, and red pigments. Science 232, 193–202 (1986) | Article | PubMed | ISI | ChemPort |
  20. Neitz, M., Neitz, J. & Jacobs, G. H. Spectral tuning of pigments underlying red-green color vision. Science 252, 971–974 (1991) | Article | PubMed | ISI | ChemPort |
  21. Büning, H., Perabo, L., Coutelle, O., Quadt-Humme, S. & Hallek, M. Recent developments in adeno-associated virus vector technology. J. Gene Med. 10, 717–733 (2008) | Article | PubMed | ChemPort |
Top

Acknowledgements

This work was supported by the National Institutes of Health grants R01EY016861 (M.N.) and R01EY11123 (W.W.H.); Research Training Program in Vision Science Grant T32EY014537; NEI Core Grants for Vision Research P30EY01931, P30EY01730 and P30EY08571; the Harry J. Heeb Foundation, the Posner Foundation, the Macular Vision Research Foundation, the Foundation Fighting Blindness, Hope for Vision, and Research to Prevent Blindness. We would like to thank V. Chiodo, S. Boye, D. Conklyn, P. M. Summerfelt, K. Chmielewski and K. L. Gunther for technical assistance. J.N. is the Bishop Professor in Ophthalmology, M.N. is the Ray Hill Professor in Ophthalmology, and W.W.H. is Rybaczki-Bullard Professor of Ophthalmology.

Author Contributions Experiments and data analysis were performed by K.M., T.B.C., J.A.K., M.C.M., J.N. and M.N. Cone-specific expression of the gene therapy vector was developed and validated by Q.L., and W.W.H. constructed the vector and packaged it into adeno-associated virus and provided dosage guidance. All authors contributed to data interpretation. The manuscript was written by K.M., J.N. and M.N. and incorporates comments by all others.

Top

Competing interests statement

The authors declare  competing financial interests.

Top

Online Methods

Viral vector

CHOPS2053 was a 2.1-kilobase (kb) fragment containing the locus control region and proximal promoter upstream of the human X-chromosome opsin gene array9, 17. These elements (also known as pR2.1) have been shown to target transgene expression to mammalian L/M cones3, 18. RHLOPS was a 1.2-kb fragment containing recombinant human L-opsin cDNA. A clone of the human L-opsin cDNA19, known as hs7, was generously provided by J. Nathans. The QuickChange kit (Stratagene) was used to convert codon180 so that it would encode a human L pigment maximally sensitive to 562nm20. The virus was made using the genome from rAAV serotype 2 and the capsid from serotype 5, and the preparation had 9×1013 DNase-resistant vector genome containing particles per ml. To prevent vector aggregation, 0.014%Tween-20 was added to the final vector preparation. A total of 2.7×1013 viral particles were injected per eye.

An earlier version of the L-opsin-coding rAAV2/5 used in previous unsuccessful experiments contained an extra 146-bp segment between the splice donor/acceptor site and the translational start codon of the L-opsin gene that had been carried over from the cloning vector. Because we were concerned that this fragment may have interfered with transgene expression, a second version of L-opsin rAAV2/5 in which the extra 146bp had been removed was used in later experiments described here. In addition to modifying the vector, we also increased the amount of viral particles delivered per eye by approximately 16-fold, from 1.7×1012 to 2.7×1013. Thus, we cannot conclude from this set of experiments what exact titre of viral particles was required to produce the effects on colour vision behaviour, or exactly what effects, if any, the extra 146bp had on transgene expression in earlier unsuccessful attempts.

The single-stranded DNA genome of conventional rAAV vectors, including rAAV2/5 used here, is devoid of Rep coding sequences. Thus, the vector genome is stabilized predominantly in an episomal form; however, the potential for integration exists21. According to NIH guidelines, the viral vector used here is rated biosafety level 1 (BSL1), and animal biosafety level 1(ABSL1) meaning that no special precautions were required in handling the virus or animals treated with the virus. After treatment, squirrel monkeys had an increase in AAV antibody titres, ranging from 4–12-fold. Antibody titres remained unchanged in untreated control animals who were housed with treated animals.

Subretinal injections

Subretinal injections were performed by a vitreo-retinal surgeon (T.B.C.) using a KDS model 210 syringe pump under a stereomicroscope. A 500-μl Hamilton Gastight (1750TTL) Luer Lock syringe was connected to 88.9cm of 30 gauge teflon tubing with male Luer Lock adapters at both ends (Hamilton 30TF double hub), which was then connected to a 30-gauge Becton Dickinson Yale regular bevel cannula (ref 511258) that was manually bent to produce a 135° angle 1.5-mm from the tip. All components were sterilized before use. The syringe and tubing were filled with sterile lactated Ringers solution to produce a dead volume of approximately 210μl. Just before injection, 300μl of rAAV was withdrawn using a rate of 100 μlmin-1.

Squirrel monkeys were anaesthetized using intramuscular injections of ketamine (15mgkg-1) and xylazine (2mgkg-1); atropine (0.05mgkg-1) was also given to reduce airway secretions. The eye was dilated with 2–3 drops of tropicamide (1%) and treated with one drop each of betadine (5%), vigamox (0.5%) and proparacaine (1%). Subconjunctival injection of 0.1ml lidocaine (2%) was given, and the anterior portion of the eye was exposed by performing a temporal canthotomy followed by limited conjuntival peritomy. Eyelids were held open with a speculum designed for premature infants. A temporal sclerotomy was made 1-mm posterior to the limbus with a 27-gauge needle, through which the injection cannula was inserted. Three subsequent 100-μl injections were made at different subretinal locations using an infusion rate of 1,060µlmin-1. Post-procedure, 0.05ml each of decadron (10mgml-1), kenalog (40mgml-1) and cephazolin (100mgml-1) were injected subconjunctivaly; one drop each of betadine (5%) and vigamox (0.5%) and a 0.6-cm strip of tobradex (0.3% tobramycin, 0.1% dexamethasone) ointment were applied topically; 10–20ml of subcutaneous fluids (sterile lactated Ringers) was also given. Subsequent administration of steroids and analgesics was administered as needed post-procedure for potential inflammation or discomfort.

MORE ARTICLES LIKE THIS

These links to content published by NPG are automatically generated.

REVIEWS

Colour vision deficiency

Eye Review

The machinery of colour vision

Nature Reviews Neuroscience Review (01 Apr 2007)

See all 7 matches for Reviews

NEWS AND VIEWS

Vision Gene therapy in colour

Nature News and Views (08 Oct 2009)

Hue and the heptahelicals

Nature News and Views (27 Jun 1991)

See all 10 matches for News And Views

RESEARCH

Surrogate Endpoints for the Treatment of Venous Leg Ulcers

Journal of Investigative Dermatology Original Article

See all 33 matches for Research

Extra navigation

.

Open Innovation Challenges

naturejobs

ADVERTISEMENT